As our world becomes more technical, preparing students for success often means improving their spatial skills. Paradoxically, new research suggests the training should begin with the way children use words.
Hilary Miller, a University of Wisconsin-Madison graduate student studying child development, explains that it’s not how many words a kid knows, it’s how they choose them.
That is, how the words describe the child’s grasp of the layout of their physical word tells the most about their spatial skills. For example, using words to show that they understand where they are relative to a friend, explaining how to rotate puzzle pieces to fit them together, or conjuring a mental map of the park, can improve spatial skills.
“We know that better spatial abilities lead to better math skills in early childhood, and they are strong predictors of future interest in careers in science and technology and engineering,” says Miller. “So we’re targeting ways to enhance spatial skills at an early age.”
Working with UW-Madison psychology Professor Vanessa Simmering and educational psychology Professor Haley Vlach, Miller is studying the way 4-year-olds use words to describe spatial relationships.
“At that age, they may have learned words like ‘above’ and ‘below’ that adults use often to describe a location,” Miller says. “But simply knowing the words doesn’t necessarily help. They have to know when that kind of information is useful.”
In a study published in the journal Child Development, Miller shows preschool age kids often skip location words and lean on other relevant information to describe important spatial details.
Miller showed each child a series of pictures in which a mouse appeared in different spots — atop objects that shifted from picture to picture in location, size and color — and asked the kids to describe the mouse’s location.
She scored their answers based on how much relevant information they shared, weighing it against irrelevant cues (mentioning an attribute that all the objects share, for example).
While previous research has often judged children’s spatial skills based at least in part on the number of spatial words they know, Miller found her study subjects to have a range of descriptive abilities not limited by the size of their spatial vocabulary.
“They are describing where the mouse is by saying, ‘He’s on the big table,’ or ‘on the brown box,'” Miller says. “Those size and color words aren’t spatial terms in this context, but in the context of the picture they’re seeing they are really useful.”
The better the kids were at adapting to each image and supplying relevant information, the higher their scores tended to be on the tests of other spatial skills that predict future success in, say, math.
“We think it’s attention to relevant information that is propelling them to perform better, and not their ability to say particular words,” says Miller, whose work is supported by the National Institutes of Health and UW-Madison’s Waisman Center.
“We’re hoping focusing on this early in childhood will help children get to the levels of success later on that seem to promote STEM interests.”
Because children learn about language by listening to the way it is used, adults can find everyday opportunities to help them develop the tools to build their spatial skills.
“When you talk to your children, use different kinds of language,” Miller says. “Be more conscious of how useful your spatial language is.”
The more diverse and relevant the words kids hear, the more they will start to make the same useful connections with language.
“Pointing and saying, ‘There are your shoes,’ is the sort of thing parents are probably doing most often,” Miller says. “But saying something like, ‘Your shoes are on the red rug,’ focuses your child on relevant spatial information. It’s practice for them with a different way to approach a problem.”
Source: University of Washington – Madison
{ All Hypnosis Feeds
via Psych Central News http://ift.tt/15FdJya
November 30, 2016 at 12:08AM
No comments:
Post a Comment